翻訳と辞書
Words near each other
・ Discourse grammar
・ Discourse marker
・ Discourse of Power
・ Discourse on Colonialism
・ Discourse on Defilement
・ Discourse on Inequality
・ Discourse on Metaphysics
・ Discourse on Pisa
・ Discourse on Reforming the Government of Florence
・ Discourse on the Arts and Sciences
・ Discourse on the Method
・ Discourse on the Tides
・ Discourse on Voluntary Servitude
・ Discourse particle
・ Discourse relation
Discourse representation theory
・ Discourse Studies
・ Discourse topic
・ Discourse types
・ Discourse Unit
・ Discourse, Figure
・ Discourse-completion task
・ Discourses (Meher Baba)
・ Discourses of Epictetus
・ Discourses on Livy
・ Discourses on Salt and Iron
・ DISCover
・ Discover
・ Discover (album)
・ Discover (magazine)


Dictionary Lists
翻訳と辞書 辞書検索 [ 開発暫定版 ]
スポンサード リンク

Discourse representation theory : ウィキペディア英語版
Discourse representation theory

In formal linguistics, discourse representation theory (DRT) is a framework for exploring meaning under a formal semantics approach. One of the main differences between DRT-style approaches and traditional Montagovian approaches is that DRT includes a level of abstract mental representations (discourse representation structures, DRS) within its formalism, which gives it an intrinsic ability to handle meaning across sentence boundaries. DRT was created by Hans Kamp in 1981. A very similar theory was developed independently by Irene Heim in 1982, under the name of ''File Change Semantics'' (FCS).
==Discourse representation structures==
DRT uses ''discourse representation structure''s (DRS) to represent a hearer's mental representation of a discourse as it unfolds over time. There are two critical components to a DRS:
* A set of ''discourse referents'' representing entities which are under discussion.
* A set of ''DRS conditions'' representing information that has been given about discourse referents.
Consider Sentence (1) below:
:(1) A farmer owns a donkey.
The DRS of (1) can be notated as (2) below:
:(2) (farmer(x), donkey(y), owns(x,y) )
What (2) says is that there are two discourse referents, x and y, and three discourse conditions ''farmer'', ''donkey'', and ''owns'', such that the condition ''farmer'' holds of x, ''donkey'' holds of y, and ''owns'' holds of the pair x and y.
Informally, the DRS in (2) is true in a given model of evaluation if and only if there are entities in that model which satisfy the conditions. So, if a model contains two individuals, and one is a farmer, the other is a donkey, and the first owns the second, the DRS in (2) is true in that model.
Uttering subsequent sentences result in the existing DRS being updated.
:(3) He beats it.
Uttering (3) after (1) results in the DRS in (2) being updated as follows, in (4) (assuming a way to disambiguate which pronoun refers to which individual).
:(4) (farmer(x), donkey(y), own(x,y), beat(x,y) )
Successive utterances of sentences work in a similar way, although the process is somewhat more complicated for more complex sentences such as sentences containing negation, and conditionals.

抄文引用元・出典: フリー百科事典『 ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)
ウィキペディアで「Discourse representation theory」の詳細全文を読む



スポンサード リンク
翻訳と辞書 : 翻訳のためのインターネットリソース

Copyright(C) kotoba.ne.jp 1997-2016. All Rights Reserved.